рефераты конспекты курсовые дипломные лекции шпоры

Реферат Курсовая Конспект

Combinations of Sentences

Combinations of Sentences - раздел Иностранные языки, Some General Remarks § 416. The Sentence Is Usually The Limit Of Grammatical Analysis. Conrbinatio...

§ 416. The sentence is usually the limit of grammatical analysis. Conrbinations of sentences have never got adequate attention on the part of linguists. Yet the necessity of extending linguistic analysis beyond the bounds of the sentence has of late been frequently emphasized. 2

1 H. Gleason writes: "The difference between The boy ran away
and The boy did run away is not a matter of the presence or the absence
of did. Only of the stress position. Did is there only to provide a meaning­
less carrier for that stress in the required position If anything else
were available did would not occur. Compare The boy will run away.
The boy will run away".
(Op cit p 174—175).

2 See K. Pike, op. cit., p 30- "We are forced to insist that linguistic
analysis must take as part of its essential domain the treatment of
units larger than the sentence. Without these higher-level units there
are not available adequate matrices for determining sentences themselves."

See also Z. Harris. Structural Linguistics. Preface for the Fourth Impression: "Exact linguistic analysis does not go beyond the limits of a sentence: the stringent demands of its procedures are not satisfied by the relations between the sentence and its neighbors, or between parts of one sentence and parts of its neighbors. There are however structural features which extend over longer stretches of each connected


We should naturally consider the analysis of a wordin­complete without its combinability. But for some reason the combinability of sentencesis not regarded important. One might think that each sentence is an absolutely indepen­dent unit, that its forms and meanings do not depend on its neighbours in speech. But it is not so. As H. Kufner has it, "In a very real sense very few groups of words which we would unanimously punctuate as sentences can really be called com­plete or capable of standing alone ... Most of the-sentences that we speak ... are dependent on what has been said before". 1

It goes without saying that in a book of this kind the uninvestigated problem of the combinability of sentences cannot get adequate treatment. We can only point out some lines of approach.

§ 417. As we have already noted (§ 399), the demarcation line between a sentence and a combination of sentences is very vague. Some part of a simple or composite sentence may become detached from the rest and pronounced after a pause with the intonation of a separate sentence. In writing this is often marked by punctuation. Here are some examples from A Cup of Tea by Mansfield.


She'd only to cross the pavement.

Give me four bunches of those.

Give me those stumpy little tulips.


But still she waited. And that jar of roses. Those red and white ones.


The connection between such sentences is quite evident. The word-combination those red and white ones can make a communication only when combined with some sentence whose predication is understood to refer to the word-combi­nation as well.

But even in case a sentence has its own predication, it may depend on some other sentence, or be coordinated with it, or otherwise connected, so that they form a combination of sentences. In the first of the examples above this connection is expressed by the conjunction but. The following sentences are connected by the pronominal subjects.

piece of writing or talking. These can be investigated by more differen­tiated tools."

1 H. Kufner, op. cit., p. 1—2.


Rosemary had been married two years. She had a duck of a boy ... They were rich. (Mansfield). l

The sentences below are connected by what we might be tempted to call 'pronominal predicates', and by the implicit repetition of the notional predicate (group) of the first sentence.

Come home to tea with me. Why won't you? Do. (Mansfield).

The second sentence might be extended at the expense of the first into Why won't you come? or even Why won't you come home to tea with me! Similarly, the third sentence is understood by the listener as Do come, or Do come home to tea with me. .

§ 418. We find no predication in the second sentence of the following dialogue.

How is the little chap feeling?

Very sorry for himself. (Galsworthy).

But this is not a sentence of the Rain type, with a zero predication. Here we know the subject, it is the chap-oi the first sentence. And we know the structural predicate is. So the person who asked the question perceived the answer as if it had the predication fully expressed: The little chap is very sorry for himself.

Traditionally sentences like very sorry for himself, with some part (or parts) left out are called incomplete or ellipti­cal. But as a matter of fact they are quite complete in their proper places in speech. They would become incomplete only if isolated from the sentences with which they are combined in speech, i. e. when regarded as language units with only paradigmatic relations, without syntagmatic ones. 2

When a speaker combines a sentence with a previous sen­tence in speech, he often leaves out some redundant parts

1 Speaking of the 'definite restrictions on order' found in sequences
lojiger than sentences, H. Gleason writes: "For example, John came.
He went away,
might imply that John did both. But He came. John
went away,
certainly could not have that meaning." (Op. cit., p. 57.)

2 J. Hughes writes: "Meaningless expressions like I'm sorry, he
isn't. Yes, completely,
become quite easy to classify if we may count as
part of the immediate utterance anything in the utterance just made by
the previous speaker. — Is Mr B. in> I'm sorry, he isn't (in). — Was
the factory destroyed by the fire? Yes, (it was) completely (destroyed by
the fire)." (The Science of Language.
N. Y., 1962;.


that are clear from the foregoing sentence, otherwise speech would be cumbersome. A sentence is thus often reduced to one word only.

Where are you going, old man? Jericho. (Galsworthy).

What have you got there, daddiest? Dynamite. (Shaw).

Theoretically, one and the same sentence may be represented differently in speech, depending on the sentence it is combined with. Suppose, we take the sentence John returned from Mos­cow yesterday. If this sentence is to be the answer to Who returned from Moscow yesterday? it may be reduced to John. As an answer to When did John return from Moscow? it may be reduced to Yesterday. In answer to Where did John return yesterday from? it may take the form of Moscow. Thus, John. Yesterday. Moscow, may be regarded as positionally condi­tioned speech variants of a regular two-member sentence. In this they differ from one-member sentences (§ 406).

The sentence on which such a speech variant depends may be called the head-sentence of which it is an adjunct.

§ 419. The sentence-words yes and no are regularly used as adjuncts of some head-sentences.

"Have you been talking to Hilary?" "Yes." (Gals­
worthy).

"I've never really got over my first attack." — 'Wo",
said Dinny with compunction. (Ib).

In the same function we find the typically English short predications of the '/ do' type.

— "/'// go, Dinny, if Hallorsen will take me." "He
shall".
(Ib.)

Sometimes the two go together.

"He wouldn't want me." "Yes, he would." (Ib.).

– Конец работы –

Эта тема принадлежит разделу:

Some General Remarks

От авторов... Some General Remarks... Morphology...

Если Вам нужно дополнительный материал на эту тему, или Вы не нашли то, что искали, рекомендуем воспользоваться поиском по нашей базе работ: Combinations of Sentences

Что будем делать с полученным материалом:

Если этот материал оказался полезным ля Вас, Вы можете сохранить его на свою страничку в социальных сетях:

Все темы данного раздела:

SOME GENERAL REMARKS
§ 1. In this book the phoneme, the morpheme, the word,and the sentenceare regarded as the basic units of language and speech. The definitions of these units have n

INTRODUCTION
§ 6. There exist many definitions of the. term word and none of them is generally accepted. But in the majority of cases pedple actually experience no difficulty in separating one word from

THE STRUCTURE OF WORDS
§ 7. One of the main properties of a word is its double nature. It is material because it can he heard or seen, and it is immaterial or ideal as far as its meaning is concerned. We shall regard the

THE CLASSIFICATION OF WORDS
§ 19. A morpheme usually has more than one meaning. This is the case, for instance, with both the lexical and the grammatical morpheme in the word runs. The morpheme run- has the following m

THE NOUN
§ 64. As follows from our previous discussion of the parts of speech in English, the noun may be defined as a part of speech characterized by the following features: 1. The lexico-grammati

The Category of Number
§ 71. The category of number of Engl ish nouns is the system of opposemes (such as girl — girls, foot — feet, etc.) showing whether the noun stands for оде object or more than

Noun Grammemes in Speech
§ 87. An English noun lexeme may contain four words at most (boy, boys, boy's, boys'). Each of these words, as we know (§ 19), represents not only the lexeme, but a certain grammeme as well.

THE ADJECTIVE
§ 101. Adjectives are a part of speech characterized by the following typical features: 1. The lexico-grammatical meaning of 'attributes (of substances)'. It should be understood that

Adjective Grammemes in Speech
§ 110. An English adjective lexeme may contain three words at most (strong — stronger — strongest) representing three grammemes. The fourth grammeme contains words with the oblique me

THE ADVERB
§ 116. The adverb is a part of speech characterized by the following features: * 1. The lexico-grammatical meaning of "qualitative, quan­ titative or circumstantial characteri

THE NUMERAL
§ 130. The numeral as a part of speech is characterized by 1) its lexico-grammatical meaning of 'number', 2) the category of numerical qualification represented in opposemes l

PRONOUNS
, § 139. It has been shown above that words fall into classes known as parts of speech in accordance with their lexico-grammatical meanings, morphological categories, typi­cal stem-building element

Хаймович и др
      But what is more important, pronouns can be substituted «not only for nouns, but for othe

Generalizing Pronouns
§ 178. Here belong such pronouns as all, both, each, either, every and its compounds (everyone, everybody, every­thing, everywhere) which give a generalizing indication of persons, th

THE VERB
§ 187. Analysing the verb in Modern Russian, V. V. Vi-nogradov characterizes it as "the most complex and capacious part of speech" J. Likewise, A. I. Smirnitsky 2 st

The Category of Voice
§ 203. The category of voice is the system of two-member opposemes (loves — is loved, loving — being loved, to love — to be loved, has loved — has, been loved, et

The Category of Aspect
§ 218. The category of aspect is a system of two-member opposemes such as works — is working, has worked — has been working, to work — to be working showing the characte

The Indicative Mood
§ 229. The indicative mood is the basic mood of the verb. Morphologically it is the most developed system including all the categories of the verb. Semantically it is a fact mood. It serve

The Subjunctive Mood
§ 239. Probably the only thing linguists are unanimous about with regard to the subjunctive mood is that it repre­sents an action as a 'non-fact', as something imaginary, 1

The Imperative Mood
§ 247. The imperative mood represents an action as a com­mand, urging, request, exhortation addressed to one's inter­locutor^). It is a direct expression of one's will. Therefore it is much more 's

Verb Grammemes in Speech
§ 251. When used in speech a word has but one of its lexical meanings and all of its grammatical meanings (see § 21). Thus in the sentence He writes to me every week, the verb writes

Indicative Mood Grammemes
The Present, Non-Continuous, Non-Perfect § 253. As a part of the verb system it may be said to pre­sent an action in the present (present tense) unspecified as to its character (non-contin

Voice Grammemes
§ 288. In the' system of the English language the active (non-marked) and the passive (marked) members of the voice opposeme are equal in rank, mutually complementing each other. This is not the ca

Subjunctive Mood Grammemes in Speech
§ 293. As we know, there is not a single subjunctive mood grammeme which has no homonyms in the indicative or in the imperative mood (or both). The discrimination of homo-nymous grammemes rests on

Imperative Mood Grammemes in Speech
§ 302. In most cases they serve to present an order, com­mand, exhortation, request, etc. as a direct expression of the speaker's will. In certain surroundings, however, mostly in

The Gerund
§ 321. The gerund is a verbid characterized by the follow­ing features: 1. Its dual lexico-grammatical meaning of an action partially viewed as a substance. 2. The categories

THE RESPONSE WORDS
§ 334. The response-words yes and no are characterized as a separate class by a) their meaning of 'response statement', b) their negative combinability, c)

THE INTERJECTION
§ 337. The interjection is a part of speech characterized by the following features. 1. It expresses "emotions or will without naming them. 2. It has no grammatical categorie

THE PREPOSITION
§ 342. The preposition is a part of speech characterized by the following features: Kits lexico-grammatical meaning of 'relations (of sub­stances)'. 2. Its bilateral combinability

THE CONJUNCTION
§ 353. The conjunction is a part of speech characterized by the following features. 1. Its lexico-grammatical meaning of 'relations between substances, actions, properties, situations,

THE ARTICLE
§ 364. The two words a(n), the form a separate group or class characterized by a) the lexico-grammatical meaning of '(in)definiteness', •b) the right-hand combinability with nouns,

THE PARTICLE
§ 372. The particle as a part of speech is characterized by the following features: 1. Its lexico-grammatical meaning of 'emphatic specifi­ cation'. 2. Its unilateral combina,

INTRODUCTION
§ 378. The basic unit of syntax is the sentence.There exist many definitions of the sentence, but none of them is generally accepted 1. But in the majority of cas

L/j8 Хаймсшич и
tences when the predicate verb contains no morphological word-morphemes. § 392. A unit of a higher level, as we know, contains units of the next lower level. A sentence contai

THE STRUCTURE OF A SENTENCE
§ 396. As defined (§ 3), when studying the structure of a unit, we find out its components, mostly units of the next lower level, their arrangement and their functions as parts of the unit.

A. As to Their Structure
§ 403. Sentences with only one predication are called simple sentences. Those with more than one predication have usually no general name 3. We shall call them

THE SIMPLE SENTENCE PARTS OF THE SENTENCE
§ 420. Traditionally the subject and the predicate are re­garded as the primary or principal parts of the sentence and the attribute, the object and the adverbial modifier — as the secondary parts

The Predicate
§ 430. The predicate is the member of a predication con­ taining the mood and tense (or only mood) components of predicativity. " E. g. This dictionary employs a pronunciat

G) Adverbials of attending circumstances
He sank down in his chair, resting his chin in his hand. (Dreiser). - §456. Qualitative and quantitative adverbial complements are more closely connected with their headword

Attributes
§ 459. Attributes are secondary parts of the sentence serv­ing to modify nouns or noun-equivalents in whatever func­tions they are used in the sentence. In simple sentences attributes can

Extensions
§ 467. Extensions are adjuncts of adjectives, adverbs and adlinks in a sentence. They differ from complements and attributes in being usually modifiers of modifiers, or ter-tiaries,

Connectives
§ 469. Connectives are linking-words considered as a second­ary part of the sentence. They are mostly prepositions and conjunctions. She played and sang to him. (London). C

Го-Specifiers
§ 471. The peculiar combinability and functions of particles distinguish them from all other words in the sentence. They .-are not adjuncts of definite parts of speech like complements, .attributes

Parenthetical Elements
§ 473. Parenthetical elements are peculiar parts of the sentence. a) They are characterized by negative combinability with the other words of the sentence. b) They are, as it

WORD-ORDER IN SIMPLE SENTENCES 1. The Position of the Subject and the Predicate in the Sentence
§ 477. We have already dwelt upon the fact that in Modern "English syntactical relations of words in the sentence are very often indicated by the position the words occupy in the sen­tence.

II. The Position of the Object
a) The Place of the Direct Object § 479. The direct object is usually placed after the verb unless the indirect object precedes it. He offered me his help. However

THE COMPOSITE SENTENCE
§ 492. Composite sentences, as we know (§ 403), divide into compound and complex sentences. The difference between them is not only in the relations of coordination or subordina­tion, as usually st

THE COMPOUND SENTENCE
§ 496. The clauses of compound sentences are of equal rank, but usually the clause preceding the conjunction is regarded as the initial clause to which the other clause is related. These relations

THE COMPLEX SENTENCE
§ 498. The principal clauses of complex sentences are usually not classified, though their meanings are not neutral with regard to the meanings of the subordinate clauses. Cf. He will c

Complement Clauses
a) Predicative Clauses § 509. The sentence The question is where he can be found consists of the principal clause the question is and the predi­cative clause where he can

Attribute Clauses
§ 518. Attributive clauses are postpositive adjuncts of nouns. They are commonly divided into relative and a'pposi-tiveclauses. Relative clauses are introduced by pronouns (or asyn

Extension Clauses
§ 520. Extension clauses are postpositive adjuncts of adjec­tives, adverbs and adlinks. E. g. It is indeed doubtful how he had become aware that Ro

Parenthetical Clauses
§ 521. Most authors who do not regard parenthetical ele­ments as parts of the sentence treat It is past ten, I think as a simple sentence 1. We do not find this view convin

COMPOUND-COMPLEX AND COMPLEX-COMPOUND SENTENCES
§ 523. Sometimes subordination and coordination may be combined within one sentence, in which case we may have compound-complex and complex-compound sentences. 1 See M. Ganshma,

DIRECT AND INDIRECT SPEECH
§ 524. Among the composite sentences of English and other languages we find a peculiar type differing from the rest. Cf. He said, "/ love you". (Greene). ,

CONCLUSION
§ 530. We have made a survey of the morphological and syntactical systems of Modern English, resorting, where ap­propriate, to comparison with Modern Russian. Now, by way of summing up, we shall ma

Теоретическая грамматика английского языка
(на английском языке) Редактор Л. И, Кравцова Издательский редактор Л. А. Долгопятова Технический редактор Т. Л. Гарина Корректоры Л. Т. Тихонова и

Хотите получать на электронную почту самые свежие новости?
Education Insider Sample
Подпишитесь на Нашу рассылку
Наша политика приватности обеспечивает 100% безопасность и анонимность Ваших E-Mail
Реклама
Соответствующий теме материал
  • Похожее
  • Популярное
  • Облако тегов
  • Здесь
  • Временно
  • Пусто
Теги